Charles Bonnet Syndrome (CBS)

🧩 Charles Bonnet Syndrome (CBS) — aka
“Seeing things that aren’t there… while knowing they aren’t real.”
CBS is a form of visual hallucination generated by the brain in people with eye disease or vision loss, without schizophrenia or psychotic features. 🧠✨
👁️🗨️ What is Charles Bonnet Syndrome?
Charles Bonnet Syndrome (CBS) occurs when people with impaired eyesight (e.g., partial blindness, macular degeneration, cataract) experience vivid, lifelike visual hallucinations—yet they retain full insight that these images are not real.
Common reports include seeing:
- People, housemaids, children, or unfamiliar faces
- Small creatures, butterflies, flowers
- Intricate patterns, geometric shapes, or entire scenes
Crucially, these appear in one’s visual field (not as imagined thoughts), and the person knows they are illusory.
📜 Name & Historical Note
Named after Charles Bonnet (1720–1793), a Swiss philosopher-naturalist who described the phenomenon in his grandfather, who began seeing people and objects after losing vision from cataracts.
In 1760, Bonnet published the account in Essai analytique sur les facultés de l'âme, marking the medical recognition of CBS.
🧠 Neural Mechanism
CBS arises from continued activity in the visual cortex—especially the occipital lobe and temporal visual association areas—despite reduced or absent input from the eyes.
Best explained by the Deafferentation / Release Phenomenon:
When retinal input drops out, the brain lacks data, → spontaneous firing occurs → the brain “fills the gap” by generating images.
It’s analogous to phantom limb—but here it’s “phantom vision.”
🧩 Brain Regions Involved
Region | Normal Role | In CBS |
---|---|---|
Primary Visual Cortex (V1) | Early visual processing | Spontaneous activity when input is lost |
Temporal Visual Association | Object/face memory & familiarity | Recombines stored imagery into scenes |
Parietal Lobe | Spatial integration | Places images as if in real space |
Thalamus | Relays eye–brain signals | Abnormal relay may boost cortical activation |
🔬 Evidence
- fMRI/PET: Hyperactivity in occipital cortex during hallucinations, even with no retinal input.
Santhouse et al., 2000 - EEG: Abnormal visual rhythms consistent with visual release.
Burke, 2002 - Visual deprivation: Healthy volunteers in prolonged darkness report dreamlike imagery → supports the brain’s drive to generate visual content.
🎨 What the Hallucinations Are Like
- Often colorful, detailed, sometimes moving
- Duration: seconds to hours
- Complexity ranges from simple (lights, lines, patterns) to complex (people, animals, buildings, full scenes)
- Common in dim light / quiet moments (before sleep, early morning)
- Recurring motifs are frequent
- Patients say images look “as clear as real”—but they know they aren’t real (key difference from psychosis)
Patient remarks:
“I saw tiny children playing at the foot of my bed, though I was alone.”
“Some days a man with a hat stands quietly in the corner.”
Feelings: usually not frightening (insight preserved); many feel curious or amused. Some worry about “going mad,” which can delay diagnosis.
🧭 Risk Factors
Factor | Details |
---|---|
Age > 70 | Visual system decline; cortex prone to self-activation |
Eye disease | Macular degeneration, cataract, glaucoma, partial blindness |
Low stimulation / isolation | Brain fills the visual void |
Stress / fatigue | Can increase frequency |
💊 Management & Care
Education & Reassurance (most important): explain it’s not psychosis; reduce fear by clarifying the brain mechanism.
Increase visual input: turn on lights, watch TV, do visually engaging activities.
Treat the eye condition: e.g., cataract surgery, retinal care.
Medication: usually not needed; in severe distress, low-dose antipsychotics (e.g., risperidone) may be tried.
🧠 Scientific Significance
CBS shows that vision is constructed by the brain, not just received from the eyes.
The brain is an image generator that maintains a coherent inner world even when external input is sparse—linking neuroscience, psychology, and the philosophy of perception.
🔍 Quick Summary
Topic | Key Point |
---|---|
Name | Charles Bonnet Syndrome (CBS) |
Core Feature | Vivid visual hallucinations with insight preserved |
Who’s at Risk | Visual impairment, older adults |
Key Brain Areas | Occipital cortex, temporal association areas |
Mechanism | Deafferentation → cortical release → brain-generated images |
Emotional Impact | Usually calm/neutral; worry stems from misunderstanding |
Treatment | Education, improve lighting/visual stimulation, treat eye disease |
Why it Matters | Demonstrates the brain’s active role in constructing perception |
📚 Key References
- Menon, G. J. (2005). Complex visual hallucinations in the visually impaired: The Charles Bonnet syndrome. Survey of Ophthalmology, 50(3), 239–265.
- Burke, W. (2002). The neural basis of Charles Bonnet hallucinations: A hypothesis. JNNP, 73(5), 535–541.
- Santhouse, A. M., Howard, R. J., & ffytche, D. H. (2000). The neural basis of visual hallucinations in CBS. Cortex, 36(5), 659–670.
- Teunisse, R. J., et al. (1996). Visual hallucinations in psychologically normal people: Charles Bonnet’s syndrome. The Lancet, 347(9004), 794–797.
🧩 Hashtags
#NeuroNerdSociety #CharlesBonnetSyndrome #VisualHallucination #PhantomVision #Neuroscience #VisualCortex #BrainFacts #NeuroOddities #Perception #CognitiveScience #Neuropsychology #BlindButSee #Psychology #BrainIllusions #VisualNeuroscience
Comments
Post a Comment